Berliner Boersenzeitung - Wild birds analyze grunts, whistles made by human honey-hunters

EUR -
AED 3.853118
AFN 71.487088
ALL 98.351691
AMD 409.273718
ANG 1.891004
AOA 955.659295
ARS 1055.8819
AUD 1.612967
AWG 1.8909
AZN 1.791713
BAM 1.958186
BBD 2.118481
BDT 125.382746
BGN 1.954864
BHD 0.395437
BIF 3099.635186
BMD 1.049043
BND 1.413949
BOB 7.277143
BRL 6.075321
BSD 1.049278
BTN 88.459852
BWP 14.334009
BYN 3.433683
BYR 20561.242475
BZD 2.115017
CAD 1.466756
CDF 3011.802607
CHF 0.930365
CLF 0.037034
CLP 1021.883333
CNY 7.602447
CNH 7.600206
COP 4602.760013
CRC 534.751454
CUC 1.049043
CUP 27.799639
CVE 110.399493
CZK 25.312332
DJF 186.844746
DKK 7.458245
DOP 63.237038
DZD 140.21928
EGP 52.041967
ERN 15.735645
ETB 130.835383
FJD 2.383215
FKP 0.828027
GBP 0.834471
GEL 2.874889
GGP 0.828027
GHS 16.525762
GIP 0.828027
GMD 74.48204
GNF 9042.015322
GTQ 8.10187
GYD 219.525805
HKD 8.16221
HNL 26.514301
HRK 7.483095
HTG 137.717773
HUF 409.871701
IDR 16631.527459
ILS 3.830549
IMP 0.828027
INR 88.403953
IQD 1374.474436
IRR 44138.483745
ISK 144.998255
JEP 0.828027
JMD 166.522864
JOD 0.744093
JPY 161.466076
KES 135.850881
KGS 91.045812
KHR 4211.290929
KMF 495.663583
KPW 944.138287
KRW 1468.476567
KWD 0.32278
KYD 0.874399
KZT 523.928269
LAK 23043.511793
LBP 93961.950734
LKR 305.321955
LRD 188.863681
LSL 18.970511
LTL 3.097551
LVL 0.634556
LYD 5.135256
MAD 10.541642
MDL 19.181099
MGA 4903.066576
MKD 61.374869
MMK 3407.250689
MNT 3564.648001
MOP 8.408664
MRU 41.727834
MUR 49.0118
MVR 16.217958
MWK 1819.451211
MXN 21.316857
MYR 4.670377
MZN 67.044183
NAD 18.970511
NGN 1770.238816
NIO 38.607033
NOK 11.638329
NPR 141.536123
NZD 1.79454
OMR 0.403883
PAB 1.049298
PEN 3.973941
PGK 4.22715
PHP 61.811735
PKR 291.423123
PLN 4.321649
PYG 8174.959041
QAR 3.827663
RON 4.973193
RSD 116.911696
RUB 109.267171
RWF 1432.686323
SAR 3.939045
SBD 8.79471
SCR 15.772293
SDG 630.946122
SEK 11.515901
SGD 1.411939
SHP 0.828027
SLE 23.844842
SLL 21997.91181
SOS 599.641938
SRD 37.234757
STD 21713.071748
SVC 9.181185
SYP 2635.751818
SZL 18.965104
THB 36.344126
TJS 11.185153
TMT 3.67165
TND 3.328154
TOP 2.456962
TRY 36.289233
TTD 7.126818
TWD 34.022525
TZS 2779.964489
UAH 43.543546
UGX 3887.736186
USD 1.049043
UYU 44.724485
UZS 13460.397961
VES 48.842442
VND 26666.672639
VUV 124.544491
WST 2.928499
XAF 656.769623
XAG 0.03462
XAU 0.000398
XCD 2.835091
XDR 0.802578
XOF 656.757086
XPF 119.331742
YER 262.182031
ZAR 18.932178
ZMK 9442.575435
ZMW 28.933351
ZWL 337.791413
  • RIO

    0.7350

    63.085

    +1.17%

  • BTI

    -0.0150

    37.365

    -0.04%

  • SCS

    0.5450

    13.815

    +3.94%

  • GSK

    0.1800

    34.14

    +0.53%

  • CMSD

    0.1150

    24.575

    +0.47%

  • RBGPF

    -0.9500

    59.24

    -1.6%

  • BP

    -0.5050

    29.215

    -1.73%

  • RYCEF

    -0.0500

    6.75

    -0.74%

  • RELX

    -0.2000

    46.55

    -0.43%

  • AZN

    0.5150

    66.145

    +0.78%

  • BCE

    0.0400

    26.81

    +0.15%

  • VOD

    0.1500

    8.88

    +1.69%

  • CMSC

    0.1378

    24.81

    +0.56%

  • NGG

    0.0050

    63.115

    +0.01%

  • BCC

    10.1900

    153.97

    +6.62%

  • JRI

    0.1420

    13.352

    +1.06%

Wild birds analyze grunts, whistles made by human honey-hunters
Wild birds analyze grunts, whistles made by human honey-hunters / Photo: Claire Spottiswoode - University of Cambridge/AFP

Wild birds analyze grunts, whistles made by human honey-hunters

In parts of Africa, expert honey-hunters call out to a species of bird known as the greater honeyguide, which leads them to wild bee nests -- a mutually beneficial practice.

Text size:

In a new study published Thursday in Science, researchers have found that honeyguide birds in Tanzania and Mozambique distinguish between honey-hunters' calls, responding more readily to signals used in their local area.

"The assignment of meaning to arbitrary or semi-arbitrary sounds is one of the features that characterizes human language," joint lead author Claire Spottiswoode, an evolutionary biologist at the University of Cambridge, told AFP.

The new research shows this phenomenon "extends to our interactions with other species, showing how continuous we really are with the rest of the natural world."

The Hadza, a group of hunter-gatherers in northern Tanzania, use a melodic whistle to initiate a partnership with honeyguides, while the Yao people of Mozambique use a trill followed by a grunt that sounds like "brrr-hmm!"

Once a nest is found, the humans crack it open, harvesting the honey and bee larvae, while the birds feast on the exposed beeswax.

Using mathematical models and audio playback, Spottiswoode and her co-author Brian Wood, a UCLA anthropologist, studied these signals, how helpful they were for people, and their effects on the birds.

They found the honeyguide birds of Tanzania were more than three times more likely to cooperate after hearing local Hadza whistles compared to "foreign" Yao calls.

Conversely, the honeyguides of Mozambique were almost twice as likely to seek a partnership after hearing a Yao trill-grunt than a foreign Hadza whistle.

The authors called this an example of "cultural coevolution," with humans of an area more likely to be successful if they stick to the local tradition, just as the birds of that region keep their ears out for the specific local call.

As for why such stark differences arose between the communities, practical considerations may be at play.

The Hadza hunt mammals using bows and arrows, and using a bird-like whistle reduces the chances of frightening away the other prey they are also after.

The Yao, meanwhile, don't hunt mammals and their trill-grunt might be a good way of scaring off elephants or buffaloes whom they don't wish to startle in a close encounter.

"Not just among the Hadza, but in hunting cultures around the world, people use whistles as a form of encrypted communication -- to share information while avoiding detection by prey," Wood said.

- A dying practice -

How exactly honeyguides learn localized human calls is an area for future study.

Perhaps they watch and copy the behavior of older birds, or perhaps they form positive associations between the human signal and a reward.

Nor is it known just how far this partnership goes back.

Our pre-Homo sapien ancestors acquired mastery of fire and stone tools between 1.5 - 3 million years ago, respectively, and so "it's plausible that this relationship could be really ancient," said Spottiswoode.

It wasn't until a seminal study published in 1989 that the scientific world was convinced the honeyguide-honey hunter relationship was real, not just a folktale or superstition.

But the practice is slowly dying out, partly as a result of changes in the way people obtain sweet food, and partly because people are prevented from interacting with honeyguides in protected nature parks.

"The birds still call to us, but we don't necessarily follow them," said Spottiswoode.

"We should really treasure these remaining places where the relationship still thrives and where this rich interspecies culture still exists."

(Y.Berger--BBZ)